Glucostatic theory of appetite control: Difference between revisions

From Citizendium
Jump to navigation Jump to search
imported>Gareth Leng
No edit summary
imported>Gareth Leng
Line 22: Line 22:
Thus, these short-term studies suggest that glycaemic and insulinaemic responses are implicated in appetite and satiety. The relatively early decline of blood glucose level to below the baseline after high-GI meal seems to play a role in initiating hunger and appetite.
Thus, these short-term studies suggest that glycaemic and insulinaemic responses are implicated in appetite and satiety. The relatively early decline of blood glucose level to below the baseline after high-GI meal seems to play a role in initiating hunger and appetite.


==The Long-Term Control of Feeding and Energy Balance==
==Long-Term Control of Feeding and Energy Balance==
The glucostatic hypothesis represents a physiological control system that fits the criteria for controlling short-term energy consumption. Few studies have looked at the glucostatic theory and the effect of varying GI foods in the long-term, possibly due to their reliance on participants and the lack of experimental control in such a long time frame. A study by Alfenas and Mattes looked at the long-term effects on appetite of consuming high- or low-GI foods over days and weeks. Their findings suggested there were no significant differences in both glycaemic and insulinemic responses, and, in hunger, fullness, or desire to eat <ref name="pmid16123477">{{cite journal| author=Alfenas RC, Mattes RD| title=Influence of glycemic index/load on glycemic response, appetite, and food intake in healthy humans| journal=Diabetes Care| year= 2005 |volume= 28 |pages= 2123-9 |pmid=16123477 |url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/eutils/elink.fcgi?dbfrom=pubmed&tool=sumsearch.org/cite&retmode=ref&cmd=prlinks&id=16123477}} </ref>.
The glucostatic hypothesis represents a physiological control system that fits the criteria for controlling short-term energy consumption. Few studies have looked at the glucostatic theory and the effect of varying GI foods in the long-term, possibly due to their reliance on participants and the lack of experimental control in such a long time frame. A study by Alfenas and Mattes looked at the long-term effects on appetite of consuming high- or low-GI foods over days and weeks. Their findings suggested there were no significant differences in both glycaemic and insulinemic responses, and, in hunger, fullness, or desire to eat <ref name="pmid16123477">{{cite journal| author=Alfenas RC, Mattes RD| title=Influence of glycemic index/load on glycemic response, appetite, and food intake in healthy humans| journal=Diabetes Care| year= 2005 |volume= 28 |pages= 2123-9 |pmid=16123477 |url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/eutils/elink.fcgi?dbfrom=pubmed&tool=sumsearch.org/cite&retmode=ref&cmd=prlinks&id=16123477}} </ref>.
    
    

Revision as of 08:32, 1 December 2010

All unapproved Citizendium articles may contain errors of fact, bias, grammar etc. A version of an article is unapproved unless it is marked as citable with a dedicated green template at the top of the page, as in this version of the 'Biology' article. Citable articles are intended to be of reasonably high quality. The participants in the Citizendium project make no representations about the reliability of Citizendium articles or, generally, their suitability for any purpose.

Attention niels epting.png
Attention niels epting.png
This article is currently being developed as part of an Eduzendium student project in the framework of a course entitled Appetite and Obesity at University of Edinburgh. The course homepage can be found at CZ:(U00984) Appetite and Obesity, University of Edinburgh 2010.
For the course duration, the article is closed to outside editing. Of course you can always leave comments on the discussion page. The anticipated date of course completion is 01 February 2011. One month after that date at the latest, this notice shall be removed.
Besides, many other Citizendium articles welcome your collaboration!


This article is developing and not approved.
Main Article
Discussion
Related Articles  [?]
Bibliography  [?]
External Links  [?]
Citable Version  [?]
 
This editable Main Article is under development and subject to a disclaimer.

In the early twentieth century, a link was first made between blood glucose and appetite that led to the glucostatic theory of appetite control. In 1916, Carlson had suggested that plasma concentrations of glucose could serve as a signal for both meal initiation (low levels) and meal termination (high levels) [1]. But it was not until the 1950's that Mayer put forward the glucostatic hypothesis. Originally it was thought that a rise in plasma glucose, for example after a meal, was sensed by neurons in the hypothalamus. These neurons which included “glucoreceptors” then signalled for meal termination. Glucose was thus thought of as a satiety factor [2].

This theory has been debated for many years. While many studies appear to support Mayer’s hypothesis, many others do not. The theory, popular in the 1950s, was losing support by the 1980s, when scientists were beginning to think that the control of appetite was a complex mechanism that would have to depend on the integration of many signalling pathways. The glucostatic theory was not abandoned altogether, as it was still thought to be important for short-term appetite control, but newly discovered peptides such as leptin became more likely candidates for long-term control.

Physiological background

Glucose homeostasis must be finely regulated by the absorption of food and the flow of recently stored energy substances through different metabolic pathways. Especially for brain glucose, it has to be supplied continuously from the blood since there is no storage for sugar available in the brain. Changes in glucose level elicit complex neuroendocrine responses that restore blood sugar levels to the optimum range [3]. The hypothalamus and the caudal brainstem both contain important centres which are responsible for monitoring blood glucose level and regulating appetite [4]. Ritter et al. claimed that some glucoreceptor cells are located in the hindbrain. This means that the glucose sensing cells have direct access to the central nervous system and could elicit immediate responses to retain the physiological norm[5]. Catecholamine neurons in the hindbrain may help mediating responses to glucose deficiency by linking glucoreceptor cells to forebrain and spinal neurons. This enables us to stimulate behavioural and hormonal responses that elevate blood sugar level. These include increased food intake, adrenal medullary secretion, corticosterone secretion and suppression of estrous cycles. Complex behaviours involved in activities such as detection and identification of food are mainly regulated by the forebrain. The hind brain may mediate the motivation for these activities via the neuronal circuit activated by some of the glucose sensing cells. This motivation circuit would have engaged the physical sign of energy deficiency with these behaviours [6].

Glucostatic theory

Short-term control of appetite and satiety

The Glycaemic Index (GI) is a measure of the effects of glycaemic carbohydrates on postprandial blood glucose levels. Foods that are digested rapidly to produce a sharp rise in blood glucose are classified as high-GI, whereas foods that are digested and absorbed slowly have a low-GI. It has been hypothesised that the GI of food plays an important role in regulating appetite.

Recent studies suggest that low GI diets prolong satiety and thereby reduce food intake. Pre-adolescent children (9 to 12 years-old) given low-GI breakfasts ate less lunch and showed a lower hunger rating than children who had high-GI breakfasts. The weight or sex of a child did not alter the effect of the GI of the breakfasts on lunch intake. [7] In obese adolescents, low-GI meals are associated with a lower insulin response than high-GI meals. The time intervals between meals were longer in low-GI test meal group, indicating that low-GI meals increased satiety. [8] Another study by Arumugam et al. investigated the effect of variations in postprandial glycaemia and insulinaemia on subjective appetitive sensations in overweight and obese women. They modulated the rate of ingestion of a glucose beverage to examine the postprandial effects of high and low-GI meals. Sharp peaks followed by rapid decrease in glucose and insulin levels to below baseline were observed in subjects with rapid consumption while relatively stable glucose and insulin levels were seen in subjects with slow consumption. Higher hunger ratings and prospective intake were reported by the subjects with rapid consumption than those with slow consumption after meals demonstrating the positive relationship between blood glucose concentrations and satiety [9].

On the other hand, Flint et al. argue that after ingestion of various GI meals by healthy young male participants showed no association between glycaemic response and postprandial fullness though it may reduce energy intake in a subsequent meal. In contrast, insulinaemic response after the meal seemed to be positively correlated with postprandial satiety [2].

Thus, these short-term studies suggest that glycaemic and insulinaemic responses are implicated in appetite and satiety. The relatively early decline of blood glucose level to below the baseline after high-GI meal seems to play a role in initiating hunger and appetite.

Long-Term Control of Feeding and Energy Balance

The glucostatic hypothesis represents a physiological control system that fits the criteria for controlling short-term energy consumption. Few studies have looked at the glucostatic theory and the effect of varying GI foods in the long-term, possibly due to their reliance on participants and the lack of experimental control in such a long time frame. A study by Alfenas and Mattes looked at the long-term effects on appetite of consuming high- or low-GI foods over days and weeks. Their findings suggested there were no significant differences in both glycaemic and insulinemic responses, and, in hunger, fullness, or desire to eat [10].

In contrast with the short-term, the long-term control of feeding appears to involve the secretion of leptin from fat (adipose) tissue. The concentration of leptin in the circulation is proportional to the amount of fat in the body, and it alters body weight by either inhibiting the intake of food or by triggering hyperphagia. Leptin crosses the blood-brain-barrier via a receptor-mediated transport system [11]. Its receptors are highly expressed in the hypothalamus in areas such as the arcuate nucleus and in the caudal brainstem; these are both areas that are known to control feeding and energy balance. The binding of leptin to these receptors influences the peptide systems, pro-opiodmelanocortin (POMC) and neuropeptide Y (NPY), a potent feeding stimulant. POMC neurons in the arcuate nucleus produce the peptide alpha-MSH, an appetite suppressant. An increase in leptin levels is thought to inhibit the release of orexigenic peptides, such as NYP, and increase the release of alpha-MSH, which binds to melanocortin 3 receptors (MC3-R) and melanocortin 4 receptors (MC4-R), causing a decrease in feeding and regulating the long-term control of energy balance [12].

Leptin signalling

The role of insulin and ghrelin in the glucostatic theory

Both ghrelin and insulin play important roles in appetite regulation. Increase in blood glucose and insulin level both may prolong satiety by stimulating the release of leptin and suppressing that of ghrelin.

Insulin

Insulin is required by cells in order to take up and use glucose from the blood, and circulating levels of insulin are correlated with satiety and food intake. In the short-term, insulin is involved in limiting postprandial appetite [2]. However, glycaemic responses appear to be more important for this. Another study found that in obese and overweight women serum insulin levels were negatively correlated with subjects hunger ratings. They were also positively correlated to fullness. Again glucose had a bigger part to play in this than insulin. Both these studies suggest that, although other factors appear to be important, insulin may play a role in appetite control.

Conversely, other studies have found no relationship between insulin and appetite. Because of this, it is uncertain how important insulin is in the control of appetite.

Ghrelin

Ghrelin, a peptide produced in the stomach, stimulates appetite, and it has been proposed that it is involved in glucostatic signalling. Ghrelin levels peak before a meal or when a meal is expected. In rats, insulin-induced hypoglycaemia up-regulates ghrelin mRNA expression. Ghrelin secretion is also inhibited by high glucose levels. This suggests that the cells which secrete ghrelin may be sensitive to changes in plasma glucose. This mechanism may play an important role in regulating energy metabolism.

When ghrelin is deleted, blood glucose is reduced and insulin levels are increased. This loss of ghrelin increases the ability of insulin to suppress glucose production. These findings show that ghrelin is important for glucose homeostasis. However, in a study by Sun et al. it was found that these effects were not related to changes in food intake or weight. As with insulin, there is little conclusive evidence suggesting an important role for ghrelin in the glucostatic hypothesis. There are, however, some studies have shown links between the two.

Conclusion

At present, though there has been several studies supporting the glucostatic theory in a short term, the GI of food modulating appetite and satiety in a longer period of time is uncertain. The relationship between insulin and anorexigenic leptin and orexigenic ghrelin is unclear and whether their levels are affected by GI values are difficult to simply conclude. One of the major factors that could influence these results is individual differences in the rate of digestion. Thus, this area of the field needs to be further long term investigation, encompassing the individual variance to be able to conclude that GI value is a valid predictor of appetite and satiety.



References

  1. Mobbs CV et al. (2005). "Impaired glucose signaling as a cause of obesity and the metabolic syndrome: the glucoadipostatic hypothesis". Physiol Behav 85: 3-23. DOI:10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.04.005. PMID 15924903. Research Blogging.
  2. 2.0 2.1 2.2 Flint Aet al. (2006). "Glycemic and insulinemic responses as determinants of appetite in humans". Am J Clin Nutr 84: 1365-73. PMID 17158418. Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; name "pmid17158418" defined multiple times with different content Cite error: Invalid <ref> tag; name "pmid17158418" defined multiple times with different content
  3. Ritter S et al. (2006). "Hindbrain catecholamine neurons control multiple glucoregulatory responses.". Physiol Behav 89: 490-500. DOI:10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.05.036. PMID 16887153. Research Blogging.
  4. Mayer J (1955). "Regulation of energy intake and the body weight: the glucostatic theory and the lipostatic hypothesis". Ann N Y Acad Sci 63: 15-43. PMID 13249313.
  5. Ritter RC et al. (1981). "Glucoreceptors controlling feeding and blood glucose: location in the hindbrain". Science 213: 451-2. PMID 6264602.
  6. Singer LK, Ritter S (1996). "Intraventricular glucose blocks feeding induced by 2-deoxy-D-glucose but not mercaptoacetate". Physiol Behav 59: 921-3. PMID 8778887.
  7. Warren JM et al. (2003). "Low glycemic index breakfasts and reduced food intake in preadolescent children.". Pediatrics 112: e414. PMID 14595085.
  8. Ball SD et al. (2003). "Prolongation of satiety after low versus moderately high glycemic index meals in obese adolescents.". Pediatrics 111: 488-94. PMID 12612226.
  9. Arumugam V et al. (2008). "A high-glycemic meal pattern elicited increased subjective appetite sensations in overweight and obese women.". Appetite 50: 215-22. DOI:10.1016/j.appet.2007.07.003. PMID 17714828. Research Blogging.
  10. Alfenas RC, Mattes RD (2005). "Influence of glycemic index/load on glycemic response, appetite, and food intake in healthy humans". Diabetes Care 28: 2123-9. PMID 16123477.
  11. Wynne K et al. (2005). "Appetite control". J Endocrinol 184: 291-318. DOI:10.1677/joe.1.05866. PMID 15684339. Research Blogging.
  12. Ahima R, Osei SY (2004). "Leptin and appetite control in lipodystrophy". J Clin Endocrinol Metab 89: 4254-7. DOI:10.1210/jc.2004-1232. PMID 15356017. Research Blogging.